This article s lead section may be too short to adequately summarize the key points Please consider expanding the lead t
Religiosity

This article's lead section may be too short to adequately summarize the key points.(October 2020) |
The Oxford English Dictionary defines religiosity as: "Religiousness; religious feeling or belief. [...] Affected or excessive religiousness". Different scholars have seen this concept as broadly about religious orientations and degrees of involvement or commitment. The contrast between "religious" and "religiose" (superficially religious) and the concept of "strengthening" faith suggest differences in the intensity of religiosity.

90%-100% 80%-89% 70%-79% 60%-69% | 50%-59% 40%-49% 30%-39% 20%-29% | 10%-19% 0%-9% No data |
Scholars attempt to measure religiosity at the levels of individuals or groups, but differ as to what behaviors constitute religiosity.Sociologists of religion have observed that an individual's experience, beliefs, sense of belonging, and general behavior often are not congruent with their religious behavior, since there is much diversity in how one can be religious or not. Problems arise in measuring religiosity. For instance, measures of variables such as church attendance produce different results when different methods are used, such as traditional surveys as opposed to time-use surveys.
Components
The measurement of religiosity is hampered by the difficulties involved in defining what is meant by the term and what components it includes. Numerous studies have explored the different components of religiosity, with most finding some distinction between religious beliefs/doctrine, religious practice, and spirituality. When religiosity is measured, it is important to specify which aspects of religiosity are being discussed.
Numerous studies have explored the different components of human religiosity. What most have found (often using factor analysis) is that there are multiple dimensions. For instance, Marie Cornwall and colleagues identify six dimensions of religiosity based on the understanding that there are at least three components to religious behavior: knowing (cognition in the mind), feeling (effect to the spirit), and doing (behavior of the body). For each of these components of religiosity, there were two cross classifications, resulting in the six dimensions:
- Cognition
- traditional orthodoxy
- particularistic orthodoxy
- Affect
- Palpable
- Tangible
- Behavior
- religious behavior
- religious participation
Sociologists have differed over the exact number of components of religiosity. Charles Glock's five-dimensional approach (Glock, 1972: 39) was among the first of its kind in the field of sociology of religion. Other sociologists adapted Glock's list to include additional components (see for example, a six component measure by Mervin F. Verbit). Other researchers have found different dimensions, generally ranging from four to twelve components.
What most measures of religiosity find is that there is at least some distinction between religious doctrine, religious practice, and spirituality. Most dimensions of religiosity are correlated, meaning people who often attend church services (practice dimension) are also likely to score highly on the belief and spirituality dimensions. Nonetheless, an individual's scores on a measure of religiosity can vary between dimensions; they may not score high on all dimensions or low on all dimensions.
For example[original research?], an individual could accept truthfulness of the Bible (belief dimension), but never attend a church or even belong to an organized religion (practice dimension). Another example is an individual who did not accept orthodox Christian doctrines (belief dimension) but did attend a charismatic worship service (practice dimension) in order to develop his/her sense of oneness with the divine (spirituality dimension). A different individual might disavow all doctrines associated with organized religions (belief dimension), not affiliate with an organized religion or attend religious services (practice dimension), and at the same time be strongly committed to a higher power and feel that the connection with that higher power is ultimately relevant (spirituality dimension). These are explanatory examples of the broadest dimensions of religiosity and may not be reflected in specific religiosity measures.
Demographic studies often show a wide diversity of religious beliefs, belonging, and practices in both religious and non-religious populations. For instance, among Americans who are not religious and not seeking religion, 68% believe in God, 12% are atheists, 17% are agnostics. Also, 18%[who?] self-identify as religious, 37% self-identify as spiritual but not religious, and 42% self-identify as neither spiritual nor religious. Furthermore, 21%[who?] pray every day and 24% pray once a month. Global studies on religion also show diversity.
Decades of anthropological, sociological, and psychological research have established that the common assumption of "religious congruence" is rarely accurate. "Religious congruence" is the view that religious beliefs and values are tightly integrated in an individual's mind, or that religious practices and behaviors follow directly from religious beliefs, or that religious beliefs are chronologically linear and stable across different contexts. People's religious ideas are fragmented, loosely connected, and context-dependent, like their ideas in all other domains of culture and life. The beliefs, affiliations, and behaviors of any individual are complex activities that have many sources including culture. Mark Chaves gives the following examples of religious incongruence: "Observant Jews may not believe what they say in their Sabbath prayers. Christian ministers may not believe in God. And people who regularly dance for rain don't do it in the dry season."
Difficulties in measurement
Polls and surveys
Decades of anthropological, sociological, and psychological research have shown that congruence between an individual's beliefs, attitudes, and behavior concerning religion and irreligion is rare.
The reliability of any poll results, in general and specifically on religion, can be questioned due numerous factors such as:
- there have been very low response rates for polls since the 1990s
- polls consistently fail to predict government election outcomes, which signifies that polls in general do not capture the actual views of the population
- biases in wording or topic affect how people respond to polls
- polls categorize people based on limited choices
- polls often generalize broadly
- polls have shallow or superficial choices, which complicate expressing people's complex religious beliefs and practices
- interviewer and respondent fatigue is very common
Researchers also note that an estimated 20–40% of the population changes their self-reported religious affiliation/identity over time due to numerous factors and that usually it is their answers on surveys that change, not necessarily their religious practices or beliefs.
In general, polling numbers are difficult to interpret and should not be taken at face value, since people in different cultural contexts may interpret the same questions differently.
Responses to Gallup polls on religiosity vary based on how the question is worded. Since the early 2000s, Gallup has routinely asked about complex topics like belief in God using three different question wordings and they have consistently received three different percentages in the responses.
In the United States
Two major surveys in the United States, the General Social Survey (GSS) and the Cooperative Congressional Election Study (CCES), have consistently produced discrepancies between their demographic estimates on religion that amount to 8% and growing. This is due to a few factors, such as differences in question wording that impact participant responses due to "social desirability bias"; the lumping of very different groups (atheist, agnostics, nothing in particular) into singular categories (e.g., "no religion" vs "nothing in particular"); and differences in the representativeness of the samples (e.g., "nones" are more politically moderate in the GSS sample than in the CCES sample, while Protestants are more conservative in the CCES sample than in the GSS sample).
The 2008 American Religious Identification Survey (ARIS) found a difference between how people identify and what people believe. While only 0.7% of U.S. adults identified as atheist, 2.3% said there is no such thing as a god. Only 0.9% identified as agnostic, but 10.0% said there is either no way to know if a god exists or they weren't sure. Another 12.1% said there is a higher power but no personal god. In total, only 15.0% identified as Nones or No Religion, but 24.4% did not believe in the traditional concept of a personal god. The conductors of the study concluded, "The historic reluctance of Americans to self-identify in this manner or use these terms seems to have diminished. Nevertheless ... the level of under-reporting of these theological labels is still significant ... many millions do not subscribe fully to the theology of the groups with which they identify."
According to a Pew Research Center study in 2009, only 5% of the total US population did not have a belief in a god. Out of all those without a belief in a god, only 24% self-identified as "atheist", while 15% self-identified as "agnostic", 35% self-identified as "nothing in particular", and 24% identified with a religious tradition.
Gallup's editor-in-chief, Frank Newport, argues that numbers on surveys may give an incomplete picture. In his view, declines in religious affiliation or belief in God on surveys may not actually reflect real declines, but instead increased honesty to interviewers on spiritual matters due to viewpoints previously seen as deviant becoming more socially acceptable.
Censuses
Questions of religion are "marginal" in censuses, usually optional, and are left out of most censuses in most countries. Despite attempts to standardize wording, census phrasing of the religion question have not been consistent over time or from country to country, with responders understanding them in 3 different ways. Censuses aim to enumerate religious communities, not religious faith, and "as long as the censuses in more than half of the world do not ask about religion it will not be possible to tell even within the closest million the size of the different religious communities globally." Due to the complexity of measuring religious identity, censuses sometimes also overestimate groups; this was the case for Christians in Britain, as typically one person fills out the census one behalf of a household, as distinguished from surveys which ask individual adults.
Causes and correlates
Genes and environment

The contributions of genes and environment to religiosity have been quantified in studies of twins and sociological studies of welfare, availability, and legal regulations (state religions, etc.).
Koenig and colleagues reported in a 2005 research paper that between adolescence and adulthood, the contribution of genes to variation in religiosity (called heritability) increases from 12% to 44% and the contribution of shared (family) effects decreases from 56% to 18%.
A market-based theory of religious choice and governmental regulation of religion have been the dominant theories used to explain variations of religiosity between societies[clarification needed]. However, researchers Anthony Gill and Eric Lundsgaarde documented a much stronger correlation between welfare state spending and religiosity (see diagram).
Just-world fallacy
Studies have found belief in a just world to be correlated with aspects of religiosity.
Risk-aversion
Several studies have discovered a positive correlation between the degree of religiousness and risk aversion.
See also
- Demographics of atheism
- Hyperreligiosity
- Piety
- Religion and personality
- Spiritual but not religious
Demographics
- Importance of religion by country
- Religion and happiness
- Religiosity and crime
- Religiosity and education
- Religiosity and intelligence
References
- Crabtree, Steve (31 August 2010). "Religiosity Highest in World's Poorest Nations". Gallup. Retrieved 27 May 2015. (in which numbers have been rounded)
- GALLUP WorldView - data accessed on 17 January 2009
- "religiosity". Oxford English Dictionary (Online ed.). Oxford University Press. (Subscription or participating institution membership required.). The earliest recorded usage of the former meaning is from 1382 Wycliffe's Bible, and of the latter is from 1799 by William Taylor quoted in John Warden Robberds' 1843 Memoir.
- Holdcroft, Barbara (September 2006). "What is Religiosity?". Catholic Education: A Journal of Inquiry and Practice. 10 (1): 89–103.
- For example: Maclaren, Alexander (1887). "Conciliatory and Hortatory Transition to Polemics". In Nicoll, William Robertson (ed.). The Expositor's Bible. Vol. 42: Colossians and Philemon. London: Hodder and Stoughton. p. 182. Retrieved 11 July 2024.
The faith which is already the firmest, and by its firmness may gladden an Apostle, is still capable of and needs strengthening.
- Chaves, Mark (March 2010). "SSSR Presidential Address Rain Dances in the Dry Season: Overcoming the Religious Congruence Fallacy". Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion. 49 (1): 1–14. doi:10.1111/j.1468-5906.2009.01489.x.
- Rossi, Maurizio; Scappini, Ettore (June 2014). "Church Attendance, Problems of Measurement, and Interpreting Indicators: A Study of Religious Practice in the United States, 1975-2010". Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion. 53 (2): 249–267. doi:10.1111/jssr.12115. ISSN 0021-8294.
- Holdcroft, Barbara (September 2006). "What is Religiosity?". Catholic Education: A Journal of Inquiry and Practice. 10 (1): 89–103.
- Brink, T.L. 1993. Religiosity: measurement. in Survey of Social Science: Psychology, Frank N. Magill, Ed., Pasadena, CA: Salem Press, 1993, pp. 2096–2102.
- Hill, Peter C. and Hood, Ralph W. Jr. 1999. Measures of Religiosity. Birmingham, Alabama: Religious Education Press. ISBN 0-89135-106-X
- Cornwall, Marie; Albrecht, S.L.; Cunningham, P.H.; Pitcher, B.L. (1986). "The dimensions of religiosity: A conceptual model with an empirical test". Review of Religious Research. 27 (3): 226–244. doi:10.2307/3511418. JSTOR 3511418.
- Glock, C. Y. (1972). "On the Study of Religious Commitment" in J. E. Faulkner (ed.) Religion's Influence in Contemporary Society: Readings in the Sociology of Religion, Ohio: Charles E. Merril: 38–56.
- Verbit, M. F. (1970). "The components and dimensions of religious behavior: Toward a reconceptualization of religiosity". American Mosaic. 24: 39.
- Küçükcan, T (2005). "Multidimensional Approach to Religion: a way of looking at religious phenomena". Journal for the Study of Religions and Ideologies. 4 (10): 60–70.
- Küçükcan, Talip. 2000. Can Religiosity Be Measured? Dimensions of Religious Commitment: Theories Revisited.
- "American Nones: The Profile of the No Religion Population" (PDF). American Religious Identification Survey. 2008. Retrieved 2014-01-30.
- "Religion and the Unaffiliated". "Nones" on the Rise. Pew Research Center: Religion & Public Life. October 9, 2012.
- "Most of the Religiously Unaffiliated Still Keep Belief in God". Pew Research Center. November 15, 2012.
- "The Global Religious Landscape". Pew Research Center. 2012-12-18.
- Mark Chaves. "SSSR Presidential Address Rain Dances in the Dry Season: Overcoming the Religious Congruence Fallacy". Wiley. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion. doi:10.1111/j.1468-5906.2009.01489.x. Retrieved 9 December 2024.
- Wuthnow, Robert (2015). "8. Taking Stock". Inventing American Religion: Polls Surveys, and the Tenuous Quest for a Nation's Faith. Oxford University Press. ISBN 9780190258900.
- Johnson, Byron; Stark, Rodney; Bradshaw, Matt; Levin, Jeff (2022). "Are Religious "Nones" Really Not Religious?: Revisiting Glenn, Three Decades Later". Interdisciplinary Journal of Research on Religion. 18 (7).
- Holifield, E. Brooks (2015). Why Are Americans So Religious? The Limitations of Market Explanations. Religion and the Marketplace in the United States. pp. 33–60. ISBN 9780199361809. "Such numbers cannot be taken at face value. They do not simply represent the world as it is but are self-representations. The difference between how Americans and citizens of other Western nations answer pollsters’ questions is first of all about how they think of themselves and how they want to be thought of in the context in which the question is asked. It means something different to say that one is “very religious” in Picayune, Mississippi, than it does in Oslo. Someone might have many reasons to answer yes to such a question, and it might be misleading to interpret the “yes” as having one simple meaning."
- Saad, Lydia; Hrynowski, Zach (24 June 2022). "How Many Americans Believe in God?". Gallup.com. Gallup.
The answer to how many Americans believe in God depends on how the question is asked. Gallup has measured U.S. adults' belief in God three different ways in recent years, with varying results.
- Burge, Ryan P. (March 2020). "How Many "Nones" Are There? Explaining the Discrepancies in Survey Estimates". Review of Religious Research. 62 (1): 173–190. doi:10.1007/s13644-020-00400-7. S2CID 256240351.
- Barry A. Kosmin and Ariela Keysar, "Archived copy" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on April 7, 2009. Retrieved 2009-05-08.
{{cite web}}
: CS1 maint: archived copy as title (link), March 2009, American Religious Identification Survey [ARIS 2008], Trinity College. - "Not All Nonbelievers Call Themselves Atheists | Pew Research Center's Religion & Public Life Project". Pewforum.org. 2009-04-02. Retrieved 2014-02-27.
- Johnson, David (7 April 2016). "See How Americans' Belief in God Has Changed Over 70 Years". Time. Retrieved 2018-03-24.
- Thorvaldsen, Gunnar (2014). "Religion in the Census". Social Science History. 38 (1–2): 203–220. doi:10.1017/ssh.2015.16.
- Voas, David; Bruce, Steve (January 2004). "Research note: The 2001 census and christian identification in Britain". Journal of Contemporary Religion. 19 (1): 23–28. doi:10.1080/1353790032000165087. ISSN 1353-7903.
- Gill, Anthony; Erik Lundsgaarde (2004). "State Welfare Spending and Religiosity" (PDF). Comparative Political Studies. 16 (4): 399–436. doi:10.1177/1043463104046694. S2CID 145609214.
- Bouchard, TJ Jr; McGue, M; Lykken, D; Tellegen, A (Jun 1999). "Intrinsic and extrinsic religiousness: genetic and environmental influences and personality correlates". Twin Res. 2 (2): 88–98. doi:10.1375/twin.2.2.88. PMID 10480743.
- Kirk, KM; Eaves, LJ; Martin, NG (Jun 1999). "Self-transcendence as a measure of spirituality in a sample of older Australian twins". Twin Res. 2 (2): 81–7. doi:10.1375/twin.2.2.81. PMID 10480742.
- Nolan, P., & Lenski, G. E. (2010). Human societies: Introduction to macrosociology. Boulder, CO: Paradigm Publisher. ISBN 978-1594515781.
- Koenig, L. B.; McGue, M.; Krueger, R. F.; Bouchard Jr, T. J. (2005). "Genetic and environmental influences on religiousness: findings for retrospective and current religiousness ratings". Journal of Personality. 73 (2): 471–488. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00316.x. PMID 15745438.
- Begue, L (2002). "Beliefs in justice and faith in people: just world, religiosity and interpersonal trust". Personality and Individual Differences. 32 (3): 375–382. doi:10.1016/s0191-8869(00)00224-5.
- Kurst, J.; Bjorck, J.; Tan, S. (2000). "Causal attributions for uncontrollable negative events". Journal of Psychology and Christianity. 19: 47–60.
- Noussair, Charles; Stefan T. Trautmann; Gijs van de Kuilen; Nathanael Vellekoop (2013). "Risk aversion and religion" (PDF). Journal of Risk and Uncertainty. 47 (2): 165–183. doi:10.1007/s11166-013-9174-8. S2CID 54664945..
- Adhikari, Binay; Anup Agrawal (2016). "Does local religiosity matter for bank risk-taking?". Journal of Corporate Finance. 38: 272–293. doi:10.1016/j.jcorpfin.2016.01.009..
External links


- Winter, T. Kaprio J; Viken, RJ; Karvonen, S; Rose, RJ. (Jun 1999). "Individual differences in adolescent religiosity in Finland: familial effects are modified by sex and region of residence". Twin Res. 2 (2): 108–14. doi:10.1375/136905299320565979. PMID 10480745.
Author: www.NiNa.Az
Publication date:
wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library, article, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games, mobile, phone, android, ios, apple, mobile phone, samsung, iphone, xiomi, xiaomi, redmi, honor, oppo, nokia, sonya, mi, pc, web, computer
This article s lead section may be too short to adequately summarize the key points Please consider expanding the lead to provide an accessible overview of all important aspects of the article October 2020 The Oxford English Dictionary defines religiosity as Religiousness religious feeling or belief Affected or excessive religiousness Different scholars have seen this concept as broadly about religious orientations and degrees of involvement or commitment The contrast between religious and religiose superficially religious and the concept of strengthening faith suggest differences in the intensity of religiosity Results of a 2008 2009 Gallup poll on whether respondents said that religion was important in their daily life 90 100 80 89 70 79 60 69 50 59 40 49 30 39 20 29 10 19 0 9 No data Scholars attempt to measure religiosity at the levels of individuals or groups but differ as to what behaviors constitute religiosity Sociologists of religion have observed that an individual s experience beliefs sense of belonging and general behavior often are not congruent with their religious behavior since there is much diversity in how one can be religious or not Problems arise in measuring religiosity For instance measures of variables such as church attendance produce different results when different methods are used such as traditional surveys as opposed to time use surveys ComponentsThe measurement of religiosity is hampered by the difficulties involved in defining what is meant by the term and what components it includes Numerous studies have explored the different components of religiosity with most finding some distinction between religious beliefs doctrine religious practice and spirituality When religiosity is measured it is important to specify which aspects of religiosity are being discussed Numerous studies have explored the different components of human religiosity What most have found often using factor analysis is that there are multiple dimensions For instance Marie Cornwall and colleagues identify six dimensions of religiosity based on the understanding that there are at least three components to religious behavior knowing cognition in the mind feeling effect to the spirit and doing behavior of the body For each of these components of religiosity there were two cross classifications resulting in the six dimensions Cognition traditional orthodoxy particularistic orthodoxy Affect Palpable Tangible Behavior religious behavior religious participation Sociologists have differed over the exact number of components of religiosity Charles Glock s five dimensional approach Glock 1972 39 was among the first of its kind in the field of sociology of religion Other sociologists adapted Glock s list to include additional components see for example a six component measure by Mervin F Verbit Other researchers have found different dimensions generally ranging from four to twelve components What most measures of religiosity find is that there is at least some distinction between religious doctrine religious practice and spirituality Most dimensions of religiosity are correlated meaning people who often attend church services practice dimension are also likely to score highly on the belief and spirituality dimensions Nonetheless an individual s scores on a measure of religiosity can vary between dimensions they may not score high on all dimensions or low on all dimensions For example original research an individual could accept truthfulness of the Bible belief dimension but never attend a church or even belong to an organized religion practice dimension Another example is an individual who did not accept orthodox Christian doctrines belief dimension but did attend a charismatic worship service practice dimension in order to develop his her sense of oneness with the divine spirituality dimension A different individual might disavow all doctrines associated with organized religions belief dimension not affiliate with an organized religion or attend religious services practice dimension and at the same time be strongly committed to a higher power and feel that the connection with that higher power is ultimately relevant spirituality dimension These are explanatory examples of the broadest dimensions of religiosity and may not be reflected in specific religiosity measures Demographic studies often show a wide diversity of religious beliefs belonging and practices in both religious and non religious populations For instance among Americans who are not religious and not seeking religion 68 believe in God 12 are atheists 17 are agnostics Also 18 who self identify as religious 37 self identify as spiritual but not religious and 42 self identify as neither spiritual nor religious Furthermore 21 who pray every day and 24 pray once a month Global studies on religion also show diversity Decades of anthropological sociological and psychological research have established that the common assumption of religious congruence is rarely accurate Religious congruence is the view that religious beliefs and values are tightly integrated in an individual s mind or that religious practices and behaviors follow directly from religious beliefs or that religious beliefs are chronologically linear and stable across different contexts People s religious ideas are fragmented loosely connected and context dependent like their ideas in all other domains of culture and life The beliefs affiliations and behaviors of any individual are complex activities that have many sources including culture Mark Chaves gives the following examples of religious incongruence Observant Jews may not believe what they say in their Sabbath prayers Christian ministers may not believe in God And people who regularly dance for rain don t do it in the dry season Difficulties in measurementPolls and surveys Decades of anthropological sociological and psychological research have shown that congruence between an individual s beliefs attitudes and behavior concerning religion and irreligion is rare The reliability of any poll results in general and specifically on religion can be questioned due numerous factors such as there have been very low response rates for polls since the 1990s polls consistently fail to predict government election outcomes which signifies that polls in general do not capture the actual views of the population biases in wording or topic affect how people respond to polls polls categorize people based on limited choices polls often generalize broadly polls have shallow or superficial choices which complicate expressing people s complex religious beliefs and practices interviewer and respondent fatigue is very common Researchers also note that an estimated 20 40 of the population changes their self reported religious affiliation identity over time due to numerous factors and that usually it is their answers on surveys that change not necessarily their religious practices or beliefs In general polling numbers are difficult to interpret and should not be taken at face value since people in different cultural contexts may interpret the same questions differently Responses to Gallup polls on religiosity vary based on how the question is worded Since the early 2000s Gallup has routinely asked about complex topics like belief in God using three different question wordings and they have consistently received three different percentages in the responses In the United States Two major surveys in the United States the General Social Survey GSS and the Cooperative Congressional Election Study CCES have consistently produced discrepancies between their demographic estimates on religion that amount to 8 and growing This is due to a few factors such as differences in question wording that impact participant responses due to social desirability bias the lumping of very different groups atheist agnostics nothing in particular into singular categories e g no religion vs nothing in particular and differences in the representativeness of the samples e g nones are more politically moderate in the GSS sample than in the CCES sample while Protestants are more conservative in the CCES sample than in the GSS sample The 2008 American Religious Identification Survey ARIS found a difference between how people identify and what people believe While only 0 7 of U S adults identified as atheist 2 3 said there is no such thing as a god Only 0 9 identified as agnostic but 10 0 said there is either no way to know if a god exists or they weren t sure Another 12 1 said there is a higher power but no personal god In total only 15 0 identified as Nones or No Religion but 24 4 did not believe in the traditional concept of a personal god The conductors of the study concluded The historic reluctance of Americans to self identify in this manner or use these terms seems to have diminished Nevertheless the level of under reporting of these theological labels is still significant many millions do not subscribe fully to the theology of the groups with which they identify According to a Pew Research Center study in 2009 only 5 of the total US population did not have a belief in a god Out of all those without a belief in a god only 24 self identified as atheist while 15 self identified as agnostic 35 self identified as nothing in particular and 24 identified with a religious tradition Gallup s editor in chief Frank Newport argues that numbers on surveys may give an incomplete picture In his view declines in religious affiliation or belief in God on surveys may not actually reflect real declines but instead increased honesty to interviewers on spiritual matters due to viewpoints previously seen as deviant becoming more socially acceptable Censuses Questions of religion are marginal in censuses usually optional and are left out of most censuses in most countries Despite attempts to standardize wording census phrasing of the religion question have not been consistent over time or from country to country with responders understanding them in 3 different ways Censuses aim to enumerate religious communities not religious faith and as long as the censuses in more than half of the world do not ask about religion it will not be possible to tell even within the closest million the size of the different religious communities globally Due to the complexity of measuring religious identity censuses sometimes also overestimate groups this was the case for Christians in Britain as typically one person fills out the census one behalf of a household as distinguished from surveys which ask individual adults Causes and correlatesGenes and environment National welfare spending vs church attendance in Christian societies The contributions of genes and environment to religiosity have been quantified in studies of twins and sociological studies of welfare availability and legal regulations state religions etc Koenig and colleagues reported in a 2005 research paper that between adolescence and adulthood the contribution of genes to variation in religiosity called heritability increases from 12 to 44 and the contribution of shared family effects decreases from 56 to 18 A market based theory of religious choice and governmental regulation of religion have been the dominant theories used to explain variations of religiosity between societies clarification needed However researchers Anthony Gill and Eric Lundsgaarde documented a much stronger correlation between welfare state spending and religiosity see diagram Just world fallacy Studies have found belief in a just world to be correlated with aspects of religiosity Risk aversion Several studies have discovered a positive correlation between the degree of religiousness and risk aversion See alsoDemographics of atheism Hyperreligiosity Piety Religion and personality Spiritual but not religiousDemographics Importance of religion by country Religion and happiness Religiosity and crime Religiosity and education Religiosity and intelligenceReferencesCrabtree Steve 31 August 2010 Religiosity Highest in World s Poorest Nations Gallup Retrieved 27 May 2015 in which numbers have been rounded GALLUP WorldView data accessed on 17 January 2009 religiosity Oxford English Dictionary Online ed Oxford University Press Subscription or participating institution membership required The earliest recorded usage of the former meaning is from 1382 Wycliffe s Bible and of the latter is from 1799 by William Taylor quoted in John Warden Robberds 1843 Memoir Holdcroft Barbara September 2006 What is Religiosity Catholic Education A Journal of Inquiry and Practice 10 1 89 103 For example Maclaren Alexander 1887 Conciliatory and Hortatory Transition to Polemics In Nicoll William Robertson ed The Expositor s Bible Vol 42 Colossians and Philemon London Hodder and Stoughton p 182 Retrieved 11 July 2024 The faith which is already the firmest and by its firmness may gladden an Apostle is still capable of and needs strengthening Chaves Mark March 2010 SSSR Presidential Address Rain Dances in the Dry Season Overcoming the Religious Congruence Fallacy Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 49 1 1 14 doi 10 1111 j 1468 5906 2009 01489 x Rossi Maurizio Scappini Ettore June 2014 Church Attendance Problems of Measurement and Interpreting Indicators A Study of Religious Practice in the United States 1975 2010 Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 53 2 249 267 doi 10 1111 jssr 12115 ISSN 0021 8294 Holdcroft Barbara September 2006 What is Religiosity Catholic Education A Journal of Inquiry and Practice 10 1 89 103 Brink T L 1993 Religiosity measurement in Survey of Social Science Psychology Frank N Magill Ed Pasadena CA Salem Press 1993 pp 2096 2102 Hill Peter C and Hood Ralph W Jr 1999 Measures of Religiosity Birmingham Alabama Religious Education Press ISBN 0 89135 106 X Cornwall Marie Albrecht S L Cunningham P H Pitcher B L 1986 The dimensions of religiosity A conceptual model with an empirical test Review of Religious Research 27 3 226 244 doi 10 2307 3511418 JSTOR 3511418 Glock C Y 1972 On the Study of Religious Commitment in J E Faulkner ed Religion s Influence in Contemporary Society Readings in the Sociology of Religion Ohio Charles E Merril 38 56 Verbit M F 1970 The components and dimensions of religious behavior Toward a reconceptualization of religiosity American Mosaic 24 39 Kucukcan T 2005 Multidimensional Approach to Religion a way of looking at religious phenomena Journal for the Study of Religions and Ideologies 4 10 60 70 Kucukcan Talip 2000 Can Religiosity Be Measured Dimensions of Religious Commitment Theories Revisited American Nones The Profile of the No Religion Population PDF American Religious Identification Survey 2008 Retrieved 2014 01 30 Religion and the Unaffiliated Nones on the Rise Pew Research Center Religion amp Public Life October 9 2012 Most of the Religiously Unaffiliated Still Keep Belief in God Pew Research Center November 15 2012 The Global Religious Landscape Pew Research Center 2012 12 18 Mark Chaves SSSR Presidential Address Rain Dances in the Dry Season Overcoming the Religious Congruence Fallacy Wiley Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion doi 10 1111 j 1468 5906 2009 01489 x Retrieved 9 December 2024 Wuthnow Robert 2015 8 Taking Stock Inventing American Religion Polls Surveys and the Tenuous Quest for a Nation s Faith Oxford University Press ISBN 9780190258900 Johnson Byron Stark Rodney Bradshaw Matt Levin Jeff 2022 Are Religious Nones Really Not Religious Revisiting Glenn Three Decades Later Interdisciplinary Journal of Research on Religion 18 7 Holifield E Brooks 2015 Why Are Americans So Religious The Limitations of Market Explanations Religion and the Marketplace in the United States pp 33 60 ISBN 9780199361809 Such numbers cannot be taken at face value They do not simply represent the world as it is but are self representations The difference between how Americans and citizens of other Western nations answer pollsters questions is first of all about how they think of themselves and how they want to be thought of in the context in which the question is asked It means something different to say that one is very religious in Picayune Mississippi than it does in Oslo Someone might have many reasons to answer yes to such a question and it might be misleading to interpret the yes as having one simple meaning Saad Lydia Hrynowski Zach 24 June 2022 How Many Americans Believe in God Gallup com Gallup The answer to how many Americans believe in God depends on how the question is asked Gallup has measured U S adults belief in God three different ways in recent years with varying results Burge Ryan P March 2020 How Many Nones Are There Explaining the Discrepancies in Survey Estimates Review of Religious Research 62 1 173 190 doi 10 1007 s13644 020 00400 7 S2CID 256240351 Barry A Kosmin and Ariela Keysar Archived copy PDF Archived from the original PDF on April 7 2009 Retrieved 2009 05 08 a href wiki Template Cite web title Template Cite web cite web a CS1 maint archived copy as title link March 2009 American Religious Identification Survey ARIS 2008 Trinity College Not All Nonbelievers Call Themselves Atheists Pew Research Center s Religion amp Public Life Project Pewforum org 2009 04 02 Retrieved 2014 02 27 Johnson David 7 April 2016 See How Americans Belief in God Has Changed Over 70 Years Time Retrieved 2018 03 24 Thorvaldsen Gunnar 2014 Religion in the Census Social Science History 38 1 2 203 220 doi 10 1017 ssh 2015 16 Voas David Bruce Steve January 2004 Research note The 2001 census and christian identification in Britain Journal of Contemporary Religion 19 1 23 28 doi 10 1080 1353790032000165087 ISSN 1353 7903 Gill Anthony Erik Lundsgaarde 2004 State Welfare Spending and Religiosity PDF Comparative Political Studies 16 4 399 436 doi 10 1177 1043463104046694 S2CID 145609214 Bouchard TJ Jr McGue M Lykken D Tellegen A Jun 1999 Intrinsic and extrinsic religiousness genetic and environmental influences and personality correlates Twin Res 2 2 88 98 doi 10 1375 twin 2 2 88 PMID 10480743 Kirk KM Eaves LJ Martin NG Jun 1999 Self transcendence as a measure of spirituality in a sample of older Australian twins Twin Res 2 2 81 7 doi 10 1375 twin 2 2 81 PMID 10480742 Nolan P amp Lenski G E 2010 Human societies Introduction to macrosociology Boulder CO Paradigm Publisher ISBN 978 1594515781 Koenig L B McGue M Krueger R F Bouchard Jr T J 2005 Genetic and environmental influences on religiousness findings for retrospective and current religiousness ratings Journal of Personality 73 2 471 488 doi 10 1111 j 1467 6494 2005 00316 x PMID 15745438 Begue L 2002 Beliefs in justice and faith in people just world religiosity and interpersonal trust Personality and Individual Differences 32 3 375 382 doi 10 1016 s0191 8869 00 00224 5 Kurst J Bjorck J Tan S 2000 Causal attributions for uncontrollable negative events Journal of Psychology and Christianity 19 47 60 Noussair Charles Stefan T Trautmann Gijs van de Kuilen Nathanael Vellekoop 2013 Risk aversion and religion PDF Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 47 2 165 183 doi 10 1007 s11166 013 9174 8 S2CID 54664945 Adhikari Binay Anup Agrawal 2016 Does local religiosity matter for bank risk taking Journal of Corporate Finance 38 272 293 doi 10 1016 j jcorpfin 2016 01 009 External linksLook up religiosity in Wiktionary the free dictionary Wikiversity has learning resources about Religiosity Winter T Kaprio J Viken RJ Karvonen S Rose RJ Jun 1999 Individual differences in adolescent religiosity in Finland familial effects are modified by sex and region of residence Twin Res 2 2 108 14 doi 10 1375 136905299320565979 PMID 10480745